The oblique effect has an optical component

Orientation-specific contrast thresholds after correction of high-order aberrations

Ian J. Murray, Sarah L. Elliott, Aris Pallikaris, John S Werner, Stacey Choi, Humza J. Tahir

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

7 Citations (Scopus)

Abstract

Most of the high-order aberrations of the eye are not circularly symmetric. Hence, while it is well known that human vision is subject to cortically based orientation preference in cell tuning, the optics of the eye might also introduce some orientational anisotropy. We tested this idea by measuring contrast sensitivity at different orientations of sine-wave gratings when viewing through a closed-loop adaptive optics phoropter. Under aberration-corrected conditions, mean contrast sensitivity improved for all observers by a factor of 1.8× to 5×. The detectability of some orientations improved more than others. As expected, this orientation-specific effect varied between individuals. The sensitivity benefits were accurately predicted from MTF model simulations, demonstrating that the observed effects reflected the individual's pattern of high-order aberrations. In one observer, the orientation-specific effects were substantial: an improvement of 8× at one orientation and 2× in another orientation. The experiments confirm that, for conditions that are not diffraction limited, the optics of the eye introduce rotational asymmetry to the luminance distribution on the retina and that this impacts vision, inducing orientational anisotropy. These results suggest that the traditional view of meridional anisotropy having an entirely neural origin may be true for diffractionlimited pupils but that viewing through larger pupils introduces an additional orientation-specific optical component to this phenomenon.

Original languageEnglish (US)
Pages (from-to)1-12
Number of pages12
JournalJournal of Vision
Volume10
Issue number11
DOIs
StatePublished - 2010

Fingerprint

Optical Devices
Anisotropy
Contrast Sensitivity
Pupil
Retina

Keywords

  • Adaptive optics
  • Contrast sensitivity
  • Higher order aberrations
  • Spatial vision

ASJC Scopus subject areas

  • Ophthalmology
  • Sensory Systems

Cite this

The oblique effect has an optical component : Orientation-specific contrast thresholds after correction of high-order aberrations. / Murray, Ian J.; Elliott, Sarah L.; Pallikaris, Aris; Werner, John S; Choi, Stacey; Tahir, Humza J.

In: Journal of Vision, Vol. 10, No. 11, 2010, p. 1-12.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

Murray, Ian J. ; Elliott, Sarah L. ; Pallikaris, Aris ; Werner, John S ; Choi, Stacey ; Tahir, Humza J. / The oblique effect has an optical component : Orientation-specific contrast thresholds after correction of high-order aberrations. In: Journal of Vision. 2010 ; Vol. 10, No. 11. pp. 1-12.
@article{2c8b48fce36b4173bc43ca02321141ce,
title = "The oblique effect has an optical component: Orientation-specific contrast thresholds after correction of high-order aberrations",
abstract = "Most of the high-order aberrations of the eye are not circularly symmetric. Hence, while it is well known that human vision is subject to cortically based orientation preference in cell tuning, the optics of the eye might also introduce some orientational anisotropy. We tested this idea by measuring contrast sensitivity at different orientations of sine-wave gratings when viewing through a closed-loop adaptive optics phoropter. Under aberration-corrected conditions, mean contrast sensitivity improved for all observers by a factor of 1.8× to 5×. The detectability of some orientations improved more than others. As expected, this orientation-specific effect varied between individuals. The sensitivity benefits were accurately predicted from MTF model simulations, demonstrating that the observed effects reflected the individual's pattern of high-order aberrations. In one observer, the orientation-specific effects were substantial: an improvement of 8× at one orientation and 2× in another orientation. The experiments confirm that, for conditions that are not diffraction limited, the optics of the eye introduce rotational asymmetry to the luminance distribution on the retina and that this impacts vision, inducing orientational anisotropy. These results suggest that the traditional view of meridional anisotropy having an entirely neural origin may be true for diffractionlimited pupils but that viewing through larger pupils introduces an additional orientation-specific optical component to this phenomenon.",
keywords = "Adaptive optics, Contrast sensitivity, Higher order aberrations, Spatial vision",
author = "Murray, {Ian J.} and Elliott, {Sarah L.} and Aris Pallikaris and Werner, {John S} and Stacey Choi and Tahir, {Humza J.}",
year = "2010",
doi = "10.1167/10.11.10",
language = "English (US)",
volume = "10",
pages = "1--12",
journal = "Journal of Vision",
issn = "1534-7362",
publisher = "Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Inc.",
number = "11",

}

TY - JOUR

T1 - The oblique effect has an optical component

T2 - Orientation-specific contrast thresholds after correction of high-order aberrations

AU - Murray, Ian J.

AU - Elliott, Sarah L.

AU - Pallikaris, Aris

AU - Werner, John S

AU - Choi, Stacey

AU - Tahir, Humza J.

PY - 2010

Y1 - 2010

N2 - Most of the high-order aberrations of the eye are not circularly symmetric. Hence, while it is well known that human vision is subject to cortically based orientation preference in cell tuning, the optics of the eye might also introduce some orientational anisotropy. We tested this idea by measuring contrast sensitivity at different orientations of sine-wave gratings when viewing through a closed-loop adaptive optics phoropter. Under aberration-corrected conditions, mean contrast sensitivity improved for all observers by a factor of 1.8× to 5×. The detectability of some orientations improved more than others. As expected, this orientation-specific effect varied between individuals. The sensitivity benefits were accurately predicted from MTF model simulations, demonstrating that the observed effects reflected the individual's pattern of high-order aberrations. In one observer, the orientation-specific effects were substantial: an improvement of 8× at one orientation and 2× in another orientation. The experiments confirm that, for conditions that are not diffraction limited, the optics of the eye introduce rotational asymmetry to the luminance distribution on the retina and that this impacts vision, inducing orientational anisotropy. These results suggest that the traditional view of meridional anisotropy having an entirely neural origin may be true for diffractionlimited pupils but that viewing through larger pupils introduces an additional orientation-specific optical component to this phenomenon.

AB - Most of the high-order aberrations of the eye are not circularly symmetric. Hence, while it is well known that human vision is subject to cortically based orientation preference in cell tuning, the optics of the eye might also introduce some orientational anisotropy. We tested this idea by measuring contrast sensitivity at different orientations of sine-wave gratings when viewing through a closed-loop adaptive optics phoropter. Under aberration-corrected conditions, mean contrast sensitivity improved for all observers by a factor of 1.8× to 5×. The detectability of some orientations improved more than others. As expected, this orientation-specific effect varied between individuals. The sensitivity benefits were accurately predicted from MTF model simulations, demonstrating that the observed effects reflected the individual's pattern of high-order aberrations. In one observer, the orientation-specific effects were substantial: an improvement of 8× at one orientation and 2× in another orientation. The experiments confirm that, for conditions that are not diffraction limited, the optics of the eye introduce rotational asymmetry to the luminance distribution on the retina and that this impacts vision, inducing orientational anisotropy. These results suggest that the traditional view of meridional anisotropy having an entirely neural origin may be true for diffractionlimited pupils but that viewing through larger pupils introduces an additional orientation-specific optical component to this phenomenon.

KW - Adaptive optics

KW - Contrast sensitivity

KW - Higher order aberrations

KW - Spatial vision

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=78349267874&partnerID=8YFLogxK

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/citedby.url?scp=78349267874&partnerID=8YFLogxK

U2 - 10.1167/10.11.10

DO - 10.1167/10.11.10

M3 - Article

VL - 10

SP - 1

EP - 12

JO - Journal of Vision

JF - Journal of Vision

SN - 1534-7362

IS - 11

ER -