Association of white matter hyperintensity volume with decreased cognitive functioning: The Framingham Heart Study

Rhoda Au, Joseph M. Massaro, Philip A. Wolf, Megan E. Young, Alexa Beiser, Sudha Seshadri, Ralph B. D'Agostino, Charles DeCarli

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

243 Citations (Scopus)

Abstract

Objective: To examine the relationship between white matter hyperintensity (WMH) volume on magnetic resonance images and cognitive tests in a large, population-based sample. Methods: Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging and neuropsychological evaluations were performed in 1820 dementia- and stroke-free participants from the Framingham Offspring Cohort. The WMH volume relative to total cranial volume was computed; WMH volumes more than 1 SD above the age-predicted mean were defined as large. Adjusting for age, sex, education, height, and Framingham Stroke Risk Profile, we examined the relationship between WMH and 3 cognitive factors derived from a neuropsychological test battery (verbal memory, visuospatial memory and organization, and visual scanning and motor speed) and 3 individual measures of new learning, abstract reasoning, and naming. Results: Compared with those with no or little WMH volume, participants with large WMH volume performed worse on the cognitive factors of visuospatial memory and organization (P = .04) and visual scanning and motor speed (P = .01), as well as on new learning (P = .04), but not on verbal memory (P = .52). Conclusions: In this younger community-based population of nondemented individuals, those with large WMH volume, as compared with those with less or no WMH volumes, performed significantly worse in cognitive domains generally associated with frontal lobe systems and, to a lesser extent, the medial temporal area. Further study will clarify whether large WMH volume and associated cognitive impairment lead to future risk of stroke or dementia.

Original languageEnglish (US)
Pages (from-to)246-250
Number of pages5
JournalArchives of Neurology
Volume63
Issue number2
DOIs
StatePublished - Feb 2006

Fingerprint

Stroke
Dementia
Learning
White Matter
Sex Education
Neuropsychological Tests
Frontal Lobe
Population
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy
Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Verbal Memory
Offspring
Naming
Battery
Cognitive Impairment
Cohort
Evaluation
Quantitative Methods
Cognitive Dysfunction

ASJC Scopus subject areas

  • Neuroscience(all)

Cite this

Association of white matter hyperintensity volume with decreased cognitive functioning : The Framingham Heart Study. / Au, Rhoda; Massaro, Joseph M.; Wolf, Philip A.; Young, Megan E.; Beiser, Alexa; Seshadri, Sudha; D'Agostino, Ralph B.; DeCarli, Charles.

In: Archives of Neurology, Vol. 63, No. 2, 02.2006, p. 246-250.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

Au, Rhoda ; Massaro, Joseph M. ; Wolf, Philip A. ; Young, Megan E. ; Beiser, Alexa ; Seshadri, Sudha ; D'Agostino, Ralph B. ; DeCarli, Charles. / Association of white matter hyperintensity volume with decreased cognitive functioning : The Framingham Heart Study. In: Archives of Neurology. 2006 ; Vol. 63, No. 2. pp. 246-250.
@article{c270f1cdb2ba4e18b6e9addd6a1117f6,
title = "Association of white matter hyperintensity volume with decreased cognitive functioning: The Framingham Heart Study",
abstract = "Objective: To examine the relationship between white matter hyperintensity (WMH) volume on magnetic resonance images and cognitive tests in a large, population-based sample. Methods: Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging and neuropsychological evaluations were performed in 1820 dementia- and stroke-free participants from the Framingham Offspring Cohort. The WMH volume relative to total cranial volume was computed; WMH volumes more than 1 SD above the age-predicted mean were defined as large. Adjusting for age, sex, education, height, and Framingham Stroke Risk Profile, we examined the relationship between WMH and 3 cognitive factors derived from a neuropsychological test battery (verbal memory, visuospatial memory and organization, and visual scanning and motor speed) and 3 individual measures of new learning, abstract reasoning, and naming. Results: Compared with those with no or little WMH volume, participants with large WMH volume performed worse on the cognitive factors of visuospatial memory and organization (P = .04) and visual scanning and motor speed (P = .01), as well as on new learning (P = .04), but not on verbal memory (P = .52). Conclusions: In this younger community-based population of nondemented individuals, those with large WMH volume, as compared with those with less or no WMH volumes, performed significantly worse in cognitive domains generally associated with frontal lobe systems and, to a lesser extent, the medial temporal area. Further study will clarify whether large WMH volume and associated cognitive impairment lead to future risk of stroke or dementia.",
author = "Rhoda Au and Massaro, {Joseph M.} and Wolf, {Philip A.} and Young, {Megan E.} and Alexa Beiser and Sudha Seshadri and D'Agostino, {Ralph B.} and Charles DeCarli",
year = "2006",
month = "2",
doi = "10.1001/archneur.63.2.246",
language = "English (US)",
volume = "63",
pages = "246--250",
journal = "Archives of Neurology",
issn = "0003-9942",
publisher = "American Medical Association",
number = "2",

}

TY - JOUR

T1 - Association of white matter hyperintensity volume with decreased cognitive functioning

T2 - The Framingham Heart Study

AU - Au, Rhoda

AU - Massaro, Joseph M.

AU - Wolf, Philip A.

AU - Young, Megan E.

AU - Beiser, Alexa

AU - Seshadri, Sudha

AU - D'Agostino, Ralph B.

AU - DeCarli, Charles

PY - 2006/2

Y1 - 2006/2

N2 - Objective: To examine the relationship between white matter hyperintensity (WMH) volume on magnetic resonance images and cognitive tests in a large, population-based sample. Methods: Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging and neuropsychological evaluations were performed in 1820 dementia- and stroke-free participants from the Framingham Offspring Cohort. The WMH volume relative to total cranial volume was computed; WMH volumes more than 1 SD above the age-predicted mean were defined as large. Adjusting for age, sex, education, height, and Framingham Stroke Risk Profile, we examined the relationship between WMH and 3 cognitive factors derived from a neuropsychological test battery (verbal memory, visuospatial memory and organization, and visual scanning and motor speed) and 3 individual measures of new learning, abstract reasoning, and naming. Results: Compared with those with no or little WMH volume, participants with large WMH volume performed worse on the cognitive factors of visuospatial memory and organization (P = .04) and visual scanning and motor speed (P = .01), as well as on new learning (P = .04), but not on verbal memory (P = .52). Conclusions: In this younger community-based population of nondemented individuals, those with large WMH volume, as compared with those with less or no WMH volumes, performed significantly worse in cognitive domains generally associated with frontal lobe systems and, to a lesser extent, the medial temporal area. Further study will clarify whether large WMH volume and associated cognitive impairment lead to future risk of stroke or dementia.

AB - Objective: To examine the relationship between white matter hyperintensity (WMH) volume on magnetic resonance images and cognitive tests in a large, population-based sample. Methods: Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging and neuropsychological evaluations were performed in 1820 dementia- and stroke-free participants from the Framingham Offspring Cohort. The WMH volume relative to total cranial volume was computed; WMH volumes more than 1 SD above the age-predicted mean were defined as large. Adjusting for age, sex, education, height, and Framingham Stroke Risk Profile, we examined the relationship between WMH and 3 cognitive factors derived from a neuropsychological test battery (verbal memory, visuospatial memory and organization, and visual scanning and motor speed) and 3 individual measures of new learning, abstract reasoning, and naming. Results: Compared with those with no or little WMH volume, participants with large WMH volume performed worse on the cognitive factors of visuospatial memory and organization (P = .04) and visual scanning and motor speed (P = .01), as well as on new learning (P = .04), but not on verbal memory (P = .52). Conclusions: In this younger community-based population of nondemented individuals, those with large WMH volume, as compared with those with less or no WMH volumes, performed significantly worse in cognitive domains generally associated with frontal lobe systems and, to a lesser extent, the medial temporal area. Further study will clarify whether large WMH volume and associated cognitive impairment lead to future risk of stroke or dementia.

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=32944475747&partnerID=8YFLogxK

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/citedby.url?scp=32944475747&partnerID=8YFLogxK

U2 - 10.1001/archneur.63.2.246

DO - 10.1001/archneur.63.2.246

M3 - Article

C2 - 16476813

AN - SCOPUS:32944475747

VL - 63

SP - 246

EP - 250

JO - Archives of Neurology

JF - Archives of Neurology

SN - 0003-9942

IS - 2

ER -